Back to top

Is your audience receptive? The role of perceived susceptibility in message acceptance and behaviour change

Contents

I Introduction
II Susceptibility as a determinant of message acceptance and behaviour change
III How to sway perceived susceptibility
IV Practice implications
V Conclusion
VI References

--Submitted by Sophie Rosa

I Introduction

Perceived susceptibility can be defined as one’s estimation of his or her chances of developing a health condition or being harmed by something. [1,2] It is a key concept in many well-known health promotion and health information processing models, such as the Health Belief Model [2], the Risk Attitude Framework [3], Parallel Response Model [4], Protection Motivation Theory [5] and the Extended Parallel Process Model [1]. In these models, perceived susceptibility is often combined with perceived severity to create the dimension of perceived threat, which is assumed to influence how health information is processed and motivates behaviour change. [1] This article presents how susceptibility, and its role in fear appeals, has been widely demonstrated as a significant determinant of message acceptance and behavior change.

II Susceptibility as a determinant of message acceptance and behaviour change

According to the models introduced above, for a message to be accepted and processed, individuals must have a sense of being susceptible to a given risk. For instance, Gallagher et al. (2011) reported higher acceptance of breast cancer screening messages in women with a higher level of susceptibility compared to women with lower levels of perceived susceptibility. [6] In adult women, perceived susceptibility to develop breast cancer is an ever present concern, with varying levels, that has been demonstrated as a predictor for detection behaviours, such as self-examinations and mammograms. [7,8] Perceived susceptibility has also been linked to a variety of other cancer prevention behaviours. [9,10,11,12] Lower levels of perceived susceptibility were associated with lower intention to engage in genetic testing and other preventive behaviours related to genetic diseases.[13]

Current published literature offers review-level evidence and single studies that provide detail on how perceived susceptibility influences message acceptance. Moreover, this same literature demonstrates how behaviour change supports the theory that the relationship between levels of susceptibility and efficacy is a key determinant in the development of effective health messages. Efficacy is a dimension in various health promotion models that includes both response-efficacy (the belief that a given behavior change will lead to the desired outcome) and self-efficacy (the belief that one is able to adopt and maintain a new health behaviour). When both perceived threat and efficacy are high, individuals tend to adopt danger control responses such as changes in attitudes, intentions and health behaviours. [14] These danger control responses indicate message acceptance and integration. Such a phenomenon has been reported in eating disorders [15], doctor-patient interactions [16], smoking cessation activities [17] and sexual health [18] campaigns. On the other hand, when susceptibility and efficacy are low, a meta-analysis by Witte & Allen (2000) reported that individuals tend to adopt fear control behaviors such as denial, defensive avoidance and opposite responses. Fear control behaviours indicate message rejection. [14] Furthermore, when perceived threat is high and efficacy is low, individuals will also tend to reject a message and do the very opposite of the advocated behaviour. [1,19,20]

III How to sway perceived susceptibility

There are various tactics to sway the level of perceived susceptibility; namely, normalization and the use of narratives with relevant statistics, both described below.

Normalization
Health communicators can aim to develop or reinforce favorable social norms to increase perceived susceptibility. Individuals may either underestimate or overestimate the prevalence of certain risk or prevalence of certain illnesses, which in turn influences their lifestyle choices. For instance, Klein & Helweg-Larsen (1980) introduced the concept of optimistic bias, defined as “people’s tendency to think their risk is less than that of their peers.”[21] On the other hand, a literature review on drinking behaviours in college reported that students consistently overestimate their peers’ drinking levels and that informing them about the real level of drinking by their peers corrected these misperceptions and reduced alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems.[22] As such, health practitioners should assess their target audience’s level of perceived susceptibility and examine the true prevalence of a risk or disease, to then appropriately design their health messages to leverage the population’s magnetic pull towards the norm. Since people are naturally motivated to comply with norms, adjusting it can become a cost-effective tactic that leads to immediate results when an increase in perceived susceptibility is required. [23,24]

Narratives and statistics
Use of narratives or storytelling is an emerging area of health communication. Evidence suggests that narratives successfully increase perceived susceptibility since they do not give rise to defensive behaviors as much as statistics have been reported to do. [25,26] It also increases relevance of the messages, especially if the individual can model and relate to the narrator or key actor in the narrative. [25,26] However, recent studies show that a combination of both narratives and statistical evidence will generate a greater increase in perceived susceptibility, as opposed to narratives alone, as seen in Nan et. al (2015) with regards to perceived susceptibility to human papillomavirus (HPV). [27] First person narratives were also found to be more effective than third person narratives. [27] Along the same lines, Frank et  al. (2005) demonstrated that relevance of the HPV narrative and level of identification with the character were both positively associated with perceived susceptibility. [28]

Susceptibility in fear appeal models
Fear appeals have garnered significant consideration in the last few decades. In 2000, Witte and Allen published a comprehensive meta-analysis on the effectiveness of fear appeals in public health campaigns indicating that greater changes in attitudes, intentions and behaviour occurred when the messages included strong fear appeals, communicated both high susceptibility and severity, and when messages contained high efficacy components. [14] Their results showed that well-designed fear appeals can be effective regardless of demographics and personality traits. [14] However, a recent series of single studies on fear appeals published in a special issue of the journal Health Communication suggested they should not be considered the best approach for every topic and audience. These findings highlight the importance of careful audience analysis to effectively manage the combination of characteristics in a message, as well as focus testing messaging strategies and materials, when using fear appeals. [28] Two fear appeal models noticeably emerge from the body of literature: the Extended Parallel Process Model [1] and the Risk Perception Attitude Framework [3], both described below.

Extended Parallel Processing Model
The Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM) [1] integrates three earlier fear appeals models: the Fear-as-acquired Drive Model [30], the Parallel Process Model [4] and the Protection Motivation Theory. [5] The EPPM aims to explain why fear appeals succeed in certain cases and fail in others. It proposes that a health message must include, in an appropriate balance, two basic constructs: fear, which includes both susceptibility and severity, and efficacy, which includes both self-efficacy and response-efficacy. The balance between those two constructs in the health message will determine the type of response the individual will have: danger control (changes in attitudes, intentions and behaviors) or fear control responses (denial, defensive avoidance or pursuing the opposite of the advocated behaviour).

EPPM is the predominant model in existing fear appeal literature, and many meta-analyses have indicated that high threat and high efficacy together consistently lead to positive changes in attitudes, intentions and behavior change. [14,31,32,33] However, these meta-analyses also indicated that the EPPM should be used only when efficacy is high. The authors also highlight gaps in research about how threat and efficacy in health messages interact to lead to positive changes in attitudes, intentions and health behaviours. [14,31,32,33]

The Risk Perception Attitude Framework
The Risk Perception Attitude framework (RPA) [3] is an audience analysis tool that segments a target audience according to its perception of risk and its self-efficacy beliefs. It integrates both the perceived efficacy construct of the Social Cognitive Theory [34] and the perceived threat constructs of the Extended Parallel Process Model [1]. However, unlike the EPPM, this model considers perceived risk and efficacy to be psychographic variables in the individual, rather than elements of the message. It also suggests that ones’ self-efficacy moderates how risk perceptions affect self-protective behaviour.  

The RPA model categorizes or classifies individuals in one of four attitude groups. First, individuals with low risk perceptions and efficacy are described as having an attitude of “indifference”. They are not motivated to act nor do they perceive to have any control over their situation. As such, they will be unlikely to engage in any self-protecting or health behaviours. Second, individuals with high risk perceptions and efficacy have a “responsive” attitude. They appreciate the risk and feel they have control over their situation and health, so they are more likely to engage in self-protective and healthy behaviours. Third, individuals with low risk perceptions and high efficacy are categorized as having a “proactive” attitude. They have a strong sense of control, but it is not enough to motivate them into self-protective and health behaviours. Lastly, individuals with low efficacy beliefs and high risk perceptions fall into the category of “avoidance” attitudes. The fact that they are motivated to act but at the same time believe they will likely fail, or have no control over their situation, prevents them from engaging in self-protective and health behaviours.

The key objective of any health message, according to this model, is to move individuals towards a “responsive” attitude to maximize health and self-protective behaviours. This model’s effectiveness has been demonstrated in a variety of topics, namely healthy eating [35], HIV prevention [36], breast cancer prevention [37,38], workplace safety [39], food safety [40], avoidance of texting and driving [41] and diabetes awareness, management and prevention [42].

IV Practice implications

Audience segmentation
The available evidence clearly shows that susceptibility is a key determinant of message acceptance and behaviour change. As such, it should be used to segment audiences accordingly to maximize the effectiveness of health communication efforts. Clearly establishing whether segments of the population should be targeted with tailored messages that moderate perceived threat, perceived efficacy, or both will be more effective than messages that are not tailored to these dimensions.

Appropriate message framing
As described earlier, Gallagher et al. (2011) reported higher acceptance of breast cancer screening messages in women with a higher level of susceptibility to breast cancer compared to women with lower levels. [6] The authors further describe the effect of message framing on the intention to get screening and actually undergoing screening in both high and low perceived susceptibility groups. In their analysis, the authors argued that loss-framed messages were more likely to increase screening behaviours in high perceived susceptibility women since they would consider a potential positive diagnosis as risky. As such, the potential loss of their health and years to their lives would be strong motivators. As for women with lower perceived susceptibility, the authors suggested that the prospect of positive results is less evocative, and as such, loss-framed messaging would not be as motivating. Also, mammograms were regarded by a majority of women as an illness-detecting process, rather than a health-affirming one, making gain-framed messages less likely to motivate women. In women who view mammograms as health-affirming, gain-framed messages were more effective. [43]  

Fear-based appeals
Health communication efforts should pair strong fear appeals with equally, if not stronger, efficacy messaging. Both response- and self-efficacy are key elements in fear appeals’ effectiveness for generating meaningful changes in health and self-protective behaviors. Lack of the efficacy dimensions in fear appeals messaging strategies will likely lead to people denying the facts or threat and reacting against it, behaving in the opposite of what is advocated, adopting a defensive mechanism such as wishful thinking, or feeling a sense of hopelessness. [14]

V Conclusion

Susceptibility and its role in fear appeals is a key variable in health communication efforts. To ensure evidence-based messaging strategies have impact, audience analysis should include a thorough examination of the target audience’s level of perceived susceptibility as well as their levels of both response and self-efficacy. Health practitioners should carefully consider these dimensions in the formulation of their health messages and communication tactics in order to elicit appropriate risk assessment and nurture sense of efficacy in their target population.  

VI References

  1. Witte, K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the Extended Parallel Process Model. Communication Monographs. 1992; 59: 329-349.
  2. Rosenstock, I.M., Strecher, V.J., & Becker, M.H. The health belief model and HIV risk behavior change. In R.J. DiClemente and J.L. Peterson, editors. Preventing AIDS: Theories and Methods of Behavioral Intentions. Plenum Press: New York. 1994. p. 5-24.
  3. Rimal RN, Real K. Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change: use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework to understand health behaviors. Hum Commun Res. 2003; 29:370–399.
  4. Leventhal, H. Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. In L. Berkowitz, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1970. Vol. 5, pp. 119-186. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60091-X
  5. Rogers, R. W. Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: a revised theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty, editors. Social Psychophysiology: A sourcebook. New York: The Guildford Press. 1983. p. 153-176
  6. Gallagher, K. M., Updegraff, J. A., Rothman, A. J., & Sims, L. Perceived Susceptibility to Breast Cancer Moderates the Effect of Gain-and Loss-Framed Messages on Use of Screening Mammography. Health Psychology : Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2011;30(2): 145–152
  7. Han PKJ, Kobrin SC, Klein WMP, Davis WW, Stefanek M, Taplin SH. Perceived ambiguity about screening mammography recommendations: Association with future mammography uptake and perceptions. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention. 2007;16:458.
  8. Katapodi MC, Lee KA, Facione NC, Dodd MJ. Predictors of perceived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: A meta-analytic review. Preventive Medicine. 2004;38:388–402.
  9. Frank LB, Murphy ST, Chatterjee JS, Moran MB, Baezconde-Garbanati L. Telling stories, saving lives: Creating narrative health messages. Health Commun. 2015;30(2):154-63
  10. Schwartz J, Bottorff JL, Ratner PA, Gotay C, Johnson KC, Memetovic J, et al. Effect of web-based messages on girls' knowledge and risk perceptions related to cigarette smoke and breast cancer: 6-month follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res Protoc. 2014;3(3):e53.
  11. Ballard JJ. Influence of a fear appeal on colorectal cancer screening among insured adults. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering. 2013;74(3-B(E))
  12. Madden K, Nan X, Briones R, Waks L. Sorting through search results: a content analysis of HPV vaccine information online. Vaccine. 2012;30(25):3741-6.
  13. Smerecnik CM, Mesters I, Candel MJ, de Vries H, de Vries NK. Genetic health messages in the mass media: do the general public perceive non-personalized genetic health message as personally relevant? Br J Health Psychol. 2010;15(Pt 4):941-56.
  14. Witte, K & Allen, M. A Meta-Analysis of Fear Appeals: Implications for Effective Public Health Campaigns Health Education & Behavior. 2000;27(5):591-615  
  15. Smalec, J. L., & Klingle, R. S. Bulimia interventions via interpersonal influence: The role of threat and efficacy in persuading bulimics to seek help. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2000; 23:37-57.
  16. Roberto, A. J., & Goodall, C. E. Using the extended parallel process model to explain physicians’ decisions to test their patients for kidney disease. Journal of Health Communication. 2009;14: 400-412.
  17. Wong, N. C. H., & Cappella, J. N. Anti-smoking threat and efficacy appeals: Effects on smoking cessation intentions for smokers low and high in readiness to quit. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2009;37:1-20.
  18. Zhang J, Zhang D, Chock T. Effects of HIV/AIDS public service announcements on attitude and behavior: Interplay of perceived threat and self-efficacy. Social Behavior and Personality. 2014;42(5):799-809.
  19. Kleinot, M. C., & Rogers, R. W. Identifying effective components of alcohol misuse prevention programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol1982;43:802-811.
  20. Rippetoe, P. A., & Rogers, R. W. Effects of components, of protection-motivation theory on adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52:596-604.
  21. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic Optimism about Susceptibility to Health Problems. Journal of Behavioural Medicine. 1980;5:441-460.
  22. Lewis MA, Neighbors C. Social norms approaches using descriptive drinking norms education: A review of the research and personalized normative feedback. Journal of American College Health. 2006;54:213–218.
  23. Schultz P.W.; Nolan, J.M.; Cialdini, R.B.; Goldstein, N.J.; Griskevicius, V. The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:429–434.
  24. Borsari, B.; Carey, K. Descriptive and Injunctive Norms in College Drinking: A Meta-Analytic Integration. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2003;64: 331–341.
  25. Thompson T, Kreuter MW. Using Written Narratives in Public Health Practice: A Creative Writing Perspective. Prev Chronic Dis. 2014;11.
  26. de Wit JB, Das E, Vet R. What works best: objective statistics or a personal testimonial? An assessment of the persuasive effects of different types of message evidence on risk perception. Health Psychol. 2008;27(1):110-5
  27. Nan X, Dahlstrom MF, Richards A, Rangarajan S. Influence of evidence type and narrative type on HPV risk perception and intention to obtain the HPV vaccine. Health Commun. 2015;30(3):301-8.
  28. Frank LB, Murphy ST, Chatterjee JS, Moran MB, Baezconde-Garbanati L. Telling stories, saving lives: Creating narrative health messages. Health Commun. 2015;30(2):154-63.
  29. Roberto, A.J. Editor’s Note for the Extended Parallel Process Model: Two Decades Later. Health Communication. 2013; 28:1-2.
  30. Hovland, C., Janis, I., & Kelly, H. Communication and persuasion. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1953.
  31. Popova, L. (2012). The extended parallel process model: Illuminating the gaps in research. Health Education & Behavior; 39(4): 455–473.
  32. de Hoog, N., Stroebe, W., & de Wit, J. B. The impact of vulnerability to and severity of a health risk on processing and acceptance of fear-arousing communications: A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology. 2007;11(3), 258–285.
  33. Peters, G. J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., & Kok, G. Threatening communication: A critical re-analysis and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health Psychology Review; 2013a: 7(sup1): S8–S31.
  34. Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986.
  35. Sulllivan HW, Beckjord EB, Rutten LJF, Hesse BW. Nutrition-related cancer prevention cognitions and behavioral intentions: Testing the risk perception attitude framework. Health Education & Behavior. 2008;35:866–879.
  36. Rimal RN, Brown J, Mkandawire G, Folda L, Creel AH. Audience segmentation as a social marketing tool in health promotion: Use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework in HIV prevention in Malawi. American Journal of Public Health. 2009;99:2224–2229.
  37. Rimal RN, Juon HS. Use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework to understand attention paid to breast cancer information and prevention behaviors among immigrant Indian women. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2010;40:287–310.
  38. Lee SY, Hwang H, Hawkins R, Pingree S. Interplay of negative emotion and health self-efficacy on the use of health information and its outcomes. Communication Research. 2008;35:358–381.
  39. Real K. Information seeking and workplace safety: A field application of the risk perception attitude framework. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2008;36:339–359.
  40. Kennedy J, Worosz M, Todd EC, Lapinksi MK. Segmentation of US consumers based on food safety attitudes. British Food Journal. 2008;110:691–705.
  41. Megan R. Dillow , Alexander G. Walsh , Frances Spellman , Megan Quirk Testing the Risk Perception Attitude Framework in the Context of Texting While Driving. Communication Research Reports. 2015;Vol. 32.
  42. Turner MM, Rimal RN, Morrison D, Kim H. The role of anxiety in seeking and retaining risk information: Testing the risk perception attitude framework in two studies. Human Communication Research. 2006;32:130–156.
  43. Rothman AJ, Kelly KM, Hertel A, Salovey P. Message frames and illness representations: Implications for interventions to promote and sustain healthy behavior. In: Cameron LD, Leventhal H, editors. The self-regulation of health and illness behavior. Routledge; London, England: 2003. p. 278–296.